ence of the animal in its relation to nature. They differ no less profoundly in some of their habits than, for instance, the klipspringer (Oreotragus saltator) and the steenbok (Neotraginus). None the less, they are the same species, and there is no morphological reason for describing the river baboon as a ‘local variety’ in the generally understood sense. Nor is there any hereditary limitation to one environment, as there would be in all lower animals similarly placed. We established experimentally that if an infant arboreal baboon is given to a mountain troop, it is adopted and grows up with the complete knowledge necessary for it to exist in its new environment.3
Among the higher vertebrates the nature of their food supply is certainly a great element in determining the course of psychic and morphological specialisation: the aardvark (Orycteropus afer afer) - an animate digging machine, toothless, with its long sticky tongue, its wonderful instinct for locating deep termite nests in hard soil; the remarkable bodily structure of the giraffe; the conversion of the flying wings of the penguin into paddles - all were modifications selected primarily by the adoption of a special food supply, and this is the case in most higher animals. It seems also to be a general rule in nature that any sudden change of environment involving the loss of natural food supply - although food for which the organism has not been specialised may be plentiful in the new environment - means destruction.
For instance, the South African otter can, in captivity, subsist on warmblooded terrestrial animals exclusively, without ever entering water, and yet several instances came to our notice where otters were driven by drought to take up their residence at shallow inland pools without fish or crabs, and they invariably died of hunger, although small terrestrial animals abounded in these drinking places. It is hardly conceivable that structural modification in this case rendered the capture of sufficient animals to sustain life impossible. It was ‘instinct’ that stood in the way.
This example emphasises the fact that the psychic specialisation is generally more powerful in confirming an animal's reaction to a definite environment than correlated somatic modifications. It is true that species outside the order of primates have been known to adopt new habits because of a radical change of food, but all the instances I know of clearly resulted from certain definite influences that are not present in the chacma.
In this country the rhinoceros-bird, which used to relieve the now vanished thick-skinned game of ticks, has undertaken the same office for the thinnerskinned domestic animals. The removal of a tick from cattle frequently leaves a small open wound, and in such cases the bird eats the exposed flesh, often causing severe, if not fatal, injuries. From this habit it was an easy transition to the practise of attacking any open wounds on cattle. The Australian parrot which has taken to picking holes in the backs of sheep is similar. It is said the habit